Original Article
Neurobehavioral Profile of Moderate to Late Preterm Infants Admitted in a Tertiary Care Centre in South India
Authors
Sonia Khurana, PhD Scholar, Department of Physiotherapy, School of Allied Health Sciences, Manipal University,
Bhamini Krishna Rao, Professor Department of Physiotherapy, School of Allied Health Sciences, Manipal University,
Leslie Edward Lewis, Professor& Head, Unit Neonatology, Department of Pediatrics, Kasturba Medical College, Manipal University,
Ramesh Yellanthoor Bhat, Professor, Unit Neonatology, Department of Pediatrics, Kasturba Medical College, Manipal University,
Jayashree Purkayastha, Associate Professor, Unit Neonatology, Department of Pediatrics, Kasturba Medical College, Manipal University,
Asha Kamath, Associate Professor in Department of Community Medicine, Kasturba Medical College, Manipal University,
Senthil Kumaran Dharmaraj, Associate Professor Department of Physiotherapy, School of Allied Health Sciences, Manipal University, Manipal – 576104, Karnataka, India.
Address for Correspondence
Bhamini Krishna Rao, Professor, Dept. of Physiotherapy, SOAHS, Manipal University, Manipal, Karnataka- 576104,
India.
E-mail:
bhaminikr@gmail.com
Citation
Khurana S, Rao BK, Lewis LE, Bhat RY, Purkayastha J, Kamath A, Dharmaraj SK. Neurobehavioral Profile of Moderate to Late Preterm Infants Admitted in a Tertiary Care Centre in South India. Online J Health Allied Scs.
2016;15(2):1. Available at URL:
http://www.ojhas.org/issue58/2016-2-1.html
Open Access Archives
http://cogprints.org/view/subjects/OJHAS.html
http://openmed.nic.in/view/subjects/ojhas.html
Submitted: Jun 13,
2016; Accepted: Jul 16, 2016; Published: Jul 30, 2016 |
|
|
|
Introduction:
Neonatal neurobehavioral organization is a multidimensional holistic phenomenon which involves synchronous working of autonomic, sensory and motor systems to influence the developmental maturation of neonates. It represents the inherent ability of the child to interact with the external environment and learn from the environment.(1) The interplay of these subsystems supports the differentiation and modulation of CNS maturation. Any disorganization in one system is reflected in other system leading to immature neurobehavioral organisation of the infant.(2)
Gestational age (GA) is one of the major indicators of neonatal maturity and is inversely proportional to the extent of impairments suffered by preterm infants.(3) Full term infants i.e. born after 37 weeks of gestation are functionally more mature and show better cortical organization as compared to infants born at earlier gestations. Preterm infants are at risk of developing developmental delays because of immature organs, especially CNS and their inadequacy to deal with the stressful environment of the NICU.(4) Studies have also postulated that last trimester is very crucial for the “wiring” of the brain. This organizational phenomenon is responsible for the development of autonomic stability, motor maturity, state organization, attention, interaction and self-regulation.(5)
Numerous studies are available for neurobehavioral assessment at term gestational ages, but literature is extremely insufficient on assessment of preterm infants born before 36 weeks of gestation are completed. Infants born between 32 weeks 0 days of gestation to 36 week 6/7 days of gestation are classified as moderate to late preterm infants. They were historically considered mature and equivalent to full term peers until recent studies which have proved even this category of infants are at risk of short term and long term complications.(6) A standardised neurobehavioral assessment which can identify the chances of delays in this low- risk category would be a great benefit to the clinicians as well as researchers in the field. Variety of assessment tools are available to assess neurobehavior of newborn infants such as- NICU Network Neurobehavioral Scale (NNNS), Hammersmith, Assessment of Preterm Infant Behavior (APIB) and Neurobehavioral assessment of preterm infant (NAPI) etc.(7)
NAPI can be a potential instrument to identify the developmental delays in this category. It has been designed to use in preterm infants as early as from 32 weeks of gestation to term. The best feature of the test is, it has been designed in such a way that large part of it can be just completed by merely observing the infant without handling and does not require professional training. NAPI has been used in number of studies and has been found to be a valid and reliable tool to identify developmental delays. Moreover, it has been found to yield 80% of sensitivity when combined with MRI to predict the cerebral palsy in preterm infants.(8) Thus the present study was aimed to assess the neurobehavioral profile of moderate to late preterm infants. Considering, a lot of differences between the western and Asian population in terms of racial and ethnic composition, legal and health care systems, and financial entitlements that could impact current and future development, we expected to find out difference in neurobehavioral maturation of the neonates. So, the secondary objective of the study was to compare the neurobehavioral profile of western and Indian population by comparing the normative scores given in the NAPI Manual with scores attained by our population.
Methods
Design: A cross sectional study was conducted in which 60 moderate to late preterm infants were assessed for neurobehavior during their nicu stay. The study was approved by institutional research committee and Institutional ethics committee (IEC 190/2014). Written informed consent was taken from the parents before participating in the study.
Subjects: All infants born between 32 week 0/7 days to 36 week 6/7 days were screened from neonatal intensive care unit of our hospital. They were examined between 24-48 hours of delivery to ensure physiological stability and to rule out the labor related influences. Infants were recruited in five gestational categories i.e 32, 33, 34, 35 and 36 weeks. Gestational age of the infant was determined by reported onset of last menstrual period and first trimester ultrasonography. Later on modified Ballard’s score was used to arrive at the best clinical estimate of GA. Infants with congenital anomalies, severe respiratory distress, genetic or chromosomal disorders were excluded from the study.
Entire procedure was explained to the parents in detail. Demographic details regarding maternal age, maternal complications, birth weight, gender, gestational maturity, Apgar scores were collected from the hospital file.
Neurobehavioral Assessment of Preterm Infants (NAPI) form was used to evaluate the neurobehavior of preterm infants. NAPI assess neurobehavior of infant in seven clusters – scarf sign, popliteal angle, motor development and vigor, alertness and orientation, irritability, cry quality and percent asleep ratings. It is applicable from 32 - 40 weeks of gestation.
Procedure: After confirming the physiological stability and feeding status (90 minute prior to the assessment), preterm infant was assessed for all items according to the sequence given in the NAPI manual. Items were performed by a physiotherapist trained for NAPI administration and with good handling skills. A total of 41 items (7 clusters) with 14 behavioral states were recorded by the examiner. Some items- scarf sign, popliteal angle etc. were performed in the open crib with radiant warmer and others- alertness & orientation involved holding the infants on the lap. After completing the examination infant was placed back under the radiant warmer and behavioural state was recorded. Raw scores were changed into converted scores for each cluster using conversion table provided in the manual. Final scores were entered into Statistical Package for Social Sciences (SPSS) for data analysis.
Data was analysed using SPSS version 16. Descriptive statistics was used to report maternal and neonatal demographic details. One sample t test was used for normally distributed data to report the difference between Indian population and western norms as provided in NAPI Manual. For skewed data non parametric test (one sample Wilcoxon signed rank test) was used.
Results
Table 1: Baseline characteristics of the study sample (N=60) |
Maternal age (years) |
28.4±4.95 |
Gender (%) |
M:
56.7 |
F:
43.3 |
Birth weight (grams) |
1583.2 ± 304 |
Gestational maturity (%) |
AGA:
59.3 |
SGA:
41 |
Table 2: Comparison of present study mean and standard deviation of NAPI variables with standard values from NAPI Manual– parametric analysis (One sample t test) |
Variable |
Gestational age |
32 (N=13) |
33 (N=16) |
34 (N=19) |
35 (N=5) |
36 (N=7) |
Sample study |
Standard reference value |
Sample study |
Standard reference value |
Sample study |
Standard reference value |
Sample study |
Standard reference value |
Sample study |
Standard reference value |
Mean ± SD |
Mean |
Mean ± SD |
Mean |
Mean ± SD |
Mean |
Mean ± SD |
Mean |
Mean ± SD |
Mean |
Motor* |
30.1±11.2 |
47.5 |
29.8±12.1 |
51.3 |
27.2±10.2 |
57.1 |
36.3±13.7 |
60 |
34.3±10.8 |
63 |
Pop angle* |
29.4± 9.9 |
21.6 |
# |
Alertness* |
21.3±11.3 |
42.3 |
25.1±11.4 |
43 |
22.4±8.1 |
48.6 |
25.5±14.1 |
51.5 |
28.6±11.5 |
54 |
*Statistically significant p-value< 0.05, # Mean & SD not following normal distribution (Non Parametric test done) |
Table 3: Comparison of present study median and interquartile range of NAPI variables with standard values from NAPI Manual – Non parametric analysis |
Variable |
Gestational age |
32 (N=13) |
33(N= 16) |
34 (N= 19) |
35 (N= 5) |
36 (N=7) |
Sample study |
Standard reference value |
Sample study |
Standard reference value |
Sample study |
Standard reference value |
Sample study |
Standard reference value |
Sample study |
Standard reference value |
Median; IQR (Q1-Q3) |
Median |
Median; IQR (Q1-Q3) |
Median |
Median; QR (Q1-Q3) |
Median |
Median; IQR (Q1-Q3) |
Median |
Median; IQR (Q1-Q3) |
Median |
Scarf sign |
16.7; 8.3 -33.3 |
33.3 |
33.3; 20.8-33.3 |
33.3 |
33.3; 16.7- 50* |
50 |
33.3; 16.6 - 50 |
50 |
16.7; 0 -33.3* |
66.7 |
Pop angle |
-- |
33.3; 33.3-45.8 |
33.3 |
33.3;33.3-33.3 |
33.3 |
33.3; 16.6 - 50 |
33.3 |
33.3; 12.5 -41.6 |
33.3 |
Irritability |
42.8; 15.4-59.4 |
33.3 |
40.4; 20.5 -46.7 |
33.3 |
40; 27.3 – 51.2 |
33.3 |
44; 6.6- 86.4 |
36.8 |
44; 39.2 – 54.7 |
41.7 |
Cry quality |
50; 0 -50 |
37.5 |
50; 0 – 50* |
50 |
50; 0 -50* |
50 |
50; 0-50 |
50 |
50; 50 -50 |
50 |
Percent asleep |
21.4; 14.2-32.1 |
28.3 |
24.9; 8.9 – 51.8 |
28.6 |
21.4; 14.2 -35.7 |
21.2 |
7.14; 3.5 – 35.6 |
21.4 |
14.2; 0-35.8 |
15.4 |
*Statistically significant p-value<0.05, |
Discussion
The present study analysed the neurobehavioral status of moderate and late preterm infants in their initial period of development. Behavioural differences were more pronounced in preterm infants belonging to lower gestations as compared to infants in higher gestational age ranges.
Authors propose that brain maturation is a complex process which involves morphological, functional and organisation changes. Although it is a complex phenomenon yet its sequence is always organised and predictable.(9) The behavioural differences observed in the lower to higher gestational ages can be attributed to the differences in the extent of cortical organisation. Cortical growth is predominantly achieved by axonal, dendritic proliferation and cortical folding which leads to enormous increase in brain surface area in the third trimester. Studies have reported preterm infants show lesser cortical folding as compared to term infants. Thus these differences in cortical folding are responsible for neurobehavioral and neurocognitive disorders in preterm infants.(10)
Myelination is the major component in white mater maturation and enables more effective transmission of neural impulses. It starts in third trimester and continues postnatally.(11) Studies have reported that myelination follow certain rules and it has been proposed that proximal pathways myelinate prior to distal, sensory pathways prior to motor and projection fibre tracts get myelinated before association pathways.(12) A study done by Weinstein M etal reported that early maturing tracts are responsible for sensory performance and late maturing tracts are responsible for motor activities. They also concluded that discrepancies in neuromaturational changes and neuronal activity in preterm infants can lead to poor neurobehavioral development.(13) Thus the variance in neurobehavioral scores of NAPI demonstrated in our study can also be corresponded to the deficient white mater maturation in 32 weeks as compared to 36 weeks.
Secondary objective of the study was to compare NAPI scores of our babies with standardized scores presented in the manual. The results of our study revealed significant differences in performance of our infants as compared to western population. Compared to the standard scores provided in the NAPI Manual our sample displayed weaker motor control and development and less alert and oriented with lower scarf angles specifically in late preterm category (34-36 weeks). Popliteal angle, irritability and cry quality was found to be considerably same throughout all gestations and comparable with standard scores. The results of our study is in consensus with other study who also reported lower performance of study samples as compared to standard norms provided in the manual.(14)
Study samples reported lower values of scarf angles as compared to standard samples which is in consensus with other studies.(15-16) The possible explanation for lower values can be; scarf sign is the measure of passive resistance when the arm is moved towards shoulder. With increasing gestational age this resistance increases in consequence of better motor development and tone at shoulder girdle.(16) This increase in tone is the shifting paradigm from hypotonia to normal physiological flexion at term age.(17) However, preterm infants continue to exhibit lower tones as compared to full term born infants. Thus in addition to early birth, environmental factors would also have a role to play in tone development patterns. (18)
Motor quality assessment is the better indicator of motor dysfunction and is clinically more useful than assessing milestone attainment.(19) Lower motor scores observed in our study are well correlated with the findings of other studies (20). Motor development is a dynamic process, multiple factors such as insufficient postnatal weight, height, head circumference may influence the motor development of child.(21) Moreover preterm infants lack proper muscle coordination, strength, inadequate postural control, poor quality of movements, lack of antigravity control which would have contributed to lower scores across all gestations.(22)
Alertness and orientation skills which involve tracking, head turning for animate and inanimate objects is the reflection of brain development. Our study samples exhibited lower scores than standard NAPI samples. These results are contradictory to the findings obtained by Gabriel P etal who reported higher alertness and orientation scores but supported by a recent study conducted by Gorzilio DM et al.(14-23) They reported when compared between moderate to late preterm category prematurity and stress tolerated by infants is the major predictor of lower scores and it improves with increase in gestational age. Infants in NICU are more exposed to adverse stimuli. Acute stress events and minimal protection from negative stimuli are major factors responsible for lower alertness and orientation scores and leading to higher chances of attentional and perceptual problems diagnosed at later age.
Decrease in percentage sleep signifies the better arousal and active state of infant. This is in consensus with other studies who have reported increase arousal levels and decrease lethargy in premature infants as they approach term age.(24-25) Authors propose that neurological maturation, growth and decrease in environmental stressors would have contributed to better arousal.(24-26) Decrease in sleep timings are also well correlated with the emergence of feeding behaviours from 34 weeks onwards and rapid progression to enable full feeds and discharge from the hospital.(27)
In irritability, crying and popliteal angle our study infants were comparable to NAPI study samples. Results of our study exhibited moderate quality of cry in all gestations, authors propose that we should be cautious in interpreting the quality of cry using NAPI because it does not keep into consideration intensity frequency and pitch of crying which are important parameters to grade quality of crying.(28-29)
An explanation for the relatively poor scores across all domains of NAPI when compared with standard reference norms may be that the population in our study usually belongs to low socioeconomic backgrounds and diverse ethnicities factors which are associated with poor devopmental outcomes and could have resulted in lower developmental profile of our children.(30)
Results of our study showed explicitly lower scores in scarf sign, motor devlopment and vigor and alertness and orientation component especially at 34 week of gestation as compared to infants born at 32,34 week. On carefully going back to our data we found that mean maternal age in 34 week born infants was 30 .38 (range of 21 to 40 years). Since maternal age is also one of the important determinant of neurodevelopment of infant. Higher maternal age would have resulted in lower scores in 34 week category.(31)
The results of the present study highlights the need to incorporate neurobehavioral asessment as a routine standard practice in all NICU’S. Firstly it will enable the clinician to find the high risk infants and provide them early intervention services. Secondly, when parents are able to read and understand the behavioral cues communicated by their infants, it will enable them to modify the activities and support neurobehavioral organisation in better way. This in turn will increase the competency of caregivers and will promote better development of their child.
Limitations of the study
Regarding the limitations of our study authors reveal that the differences in neurobehavioral organisation observed in our study can be attributed to the small sample size used in our study as compared to large sample used for normative scores of NAPI. There was unequal distribution of infants in different gestational categories which make the comparison less reliable. We also emphasize that sample of our study was preterm infants, with maximum number of infants belonging to LBW category which itself would have resulted in lower scores.
Conclusion:
The pesent study reported the neurobehavioral profile of our population. We found significant variability in scores for motor development and vigor, alertness and orientation and scarf sign domains for our study sample as compared to western standards provided in the NAPI manual. Although we found lower scores in our study sample throughout all gestations and results are supported by various studies, still authors warrant about the generalizability of standard NAPI scores for our population. Future studies need to be conducted to establish normative values of NAPI for Indian babies for reliable comparison.
References
- Bell AF, Lucas R, White-Traut RC. Concept clarification of neonatal neurobehavioural organization. J Adv Nurs. 2008 Mar 1;61(5):570–81.
- Miller MQ, Quinn-Hurst M. Neurobehavioral assessment of high-risk infants in the neonatal intensive care unit. Am J Occup Ther. 1994;48(6):506–13.
- Vieira MEB, Linhares MBM. Developmental outcomes and quality of life in children born preterm at preschool- and school-age. J Pediatr (Rio J). 2011 Aug;87(4):281–91.
- Lester BM, Miller RJ, Hawes K, Salisbury A, Bigsby R, Sullivan MC, et al. Infant Neurobehavioral Development. Semin Perinatol. 2011 Feb;35(1):8–19.
- Blackburn S. Environmental impact of the NICU on developmental outcomes. J Pediatr Nurs. 1998 Oct;13(5):279–89.
- Gouyon J-B, Iacobelli S, Ferdynus C, Bonsante F. Neonatal problems of late and moderate preterm infants. Semin Fetal Neonatal Med. 2012 Jun;17(3):146–52.
- Brown N, Spittle A. Neurobehavioral evaluation in the preterm and term infant. Curr Pediatr Rev. 2014;10(1):65–72.
- Neurobehavioral Assessment of the Preterm Infant (NAPI) - Stanford University School of Medicine [Internet]. [cited 2016 Jun 7]. Available from: http://med.stanford.edu/NAPI/
- Stiles J, Jernigan TL. The Basics of Brain Development. Neuropsychol Rev. 2010 Dec;20(4):327–48.
- Peterson BS, Anderson AW, Ehrenkranz R, Staib LH, Tageldin M, Colson E, et al. Regional Brain Volumes and Their Later Neurodevelopmental Correlates in Term and Preterm Infants. Pediatrics. 2003 May 1;111(5):939–48.
- Huang H, Zhang J, Wakana S, Zhang W, Ren T, Richards LJ, et al. White and gray matter development in human fetal, newborn and pediatric brains. NeuroImage. 2006 Oct 15;33(1):27–38.
- Dubois J, Dehaene-Lambertz G, Perrin M, Mangin J-F, Cointepas Y, Duchesnay E, et al. Asynchrony of the early maturation of white matter bundles in healthy infants: Quantitative landmarks revealed noninvasively by diffusion tensor imaging. Hum Brain Mapp. 2008 Jan 1;29(1):14–27.
- Weinstein M, Marom R, Berger I, Ben Bashat D, Gross-Tsur V, Ben-Sira L, et al. Neonatal neuropsychology: emerging relations of neonatal sensory-motor responses to white matter integrity. Neuropsychologia. 2014 Sep;62:209–19.
- Gabriel PSZ, Formiga CKMR, Linhares MBM. PEarly neurobehavioral development of preterm infants. Psicol. Reflex. Crit. Porto Alegre 2013;26(1). Available at http://www.scielo.br/scielo.php?script=sci_arttext&pid=S0102-79722013000100022
- Brown JV, Bakeman R, Sampers JS, Korner AF, Constantinou JC, Anand KJS. Comparison at 32–37 Weeks Postconception of Infants Born 1983–1989 and 1995–2004 on the Neurobehavioral Assessment of the Preterm Infant. Infancy. 2008;13:393–409.
- Lekskulchai R, Cole J. The relationship between the scarf ratio and subsequent motor performance in infants born preterm. Pediatr Phys Ther Off Publ Sect Pediatr Am Phys Ther Assoc. 2000;12(4):150–7.
- Amiel-Tison C. Clinical neurology in neonatal units. Croat Med J. 1998 Jun;39(2):136–46.
- Pineda RG, Tjoeng TH, Vavasseur C, Kidokoro H, Neil JJ, Inder T. Patterns of altered neurobehavior in preterm infants within the neonatal intensive care unit. J Pediatr. 2013 Mar;162(3):470–6.e1.
- Wolf MJ, Koldewijn K, Beelen A, Smit B, Hedlund R, de Groot IJM. Neurobehavioral and developmental profile of very low birthweight preterm infants in early infancy. Acta Paediatr2002;91(8):930–8.
- Barbosa VC, Formiga C, Linhares MBM. Assessment of the clinical and neurobehavioral variables of preterm newborns. Braz J Phys Ther. 2007 Aug;11(4):275–81.
- Marlow N. Neurocognitive outcome after very preterm birth. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. 2004 May;89(3):F224–8.
- Keller H, Bar-Or O, Kriemler S, Ayub BV, Saigal S. Anaerobic performance in 5- to 7-yr-old children of low birthweight. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2000 Feb;32(2):278–83.
- Gorzilio DM, Garrido E, Gaspardo CM, Martinez FE, Linhares MBM. Neurobehavioral development prior to term-age of preterm infants and acute stressful events during neonatal hospitalization. Early Hum Dev. 2015 Dec;91(12):769–75.
- Als H, Brazelton TB. A New Model of Assessing the Behavioral Organization in Preterm and Fullterm Infants: Two Case Studies. J Am Acad Child Psychiatry. 1981 Mar 1;20(2):239–63.
- Als H. Neurobehavioral organization of the newborn: opportunity for assessment and intervention. NIDA Res Monogr. 1991;114:106–16.
- Als H. Developmental care in the newborn intensive care unit. Curr Opin Pediatr. 1998 Apr;10(2):138–42.
- Thoyre SM. Developmental transition from gavage to oral feeding in the preterm infant. Annu Rev Nurs Res. 2003;21:61–92.
- Rautava L, Lempinen A, Ojala S, Parkkola R, Rikalainen H, Lapinleimu H, et al. Acoustic quality of cry in very-low-birth-weight infants at the age of 1 1/2 years. Early Hum Dev. 2007 Jan;83(1):5–12.
- Rothgänger H. Analysis of the sounds of the child in the first year of age and a comparison to the language. Early Hum Dev. 2003 Dec;75(1-2):55–69.
- Vohr BR, Wright LL, Dusick AM, Mele L, Verter J, Steichen JJ, et al. Neurodevelopmental and functional outcomes of extremely low birth weight infants in the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Neonatal Research Network, 1993-1994. Pediatrics. 2000 Jun;105(6):1216–26.
- Ketterlinus RD, Henderson SH, Lamb ME. Maternal age, sociodemographics, prenatal health and behavior: Influences on neonatal risk status. J Adolesc Health Care. 1990 Sep 1;11(5):423–31.
|