Introduction
Children's
social cognition, an ability to understand and
infer the thoughts and feelings of others
influences how they develop a unique view of the
world [1]. Higher social cognitive capacities are
associated with better prosocial skills whereas
difficulties in understanding and thinking about
others’ mental states can lead to disruptive
social functioning and increase the risk of major
mental health problems [2]. Examining
developmental factors that impact young children's
disruptive behaviour, we must understand that both
social and cognitive domains play an important
role in children who are suffering from disruptive
problems.
Disruptive behaviour
has been recognized as one of the most common and
persistent forms of childhood maladjustment, and
predictive of a range of negative adolescent and
adult outcomes including continuing aggression,
failure in school and work settings, substance
abuse, and late-onset psychopathology [3].
Disruptive behaviours refer to childhood problems
defined by a broad array of disruptive antisocial
behaviours [4]. Defining characteristics of
disruptive behaviour include being aggressive,
defiant, and antisocial behaviours tantrums,
oppositionality and restless, impulsive behaviours
which violate the rights of others or even
criminality. Several clinically recognized
disorders such as attention-deficit/ hyperactivity
disorder (ADHD), conduct disorder (CD), and
oppositional defiant disorder (ODD) also involve
disruptive behaviours [4]. The behaviours
characteristic of these disorders is prevalent and
serious childhood problems [4]. Recent studies
suggest that the prevalence of disruptive
behaviours is on the rise [5, 6]. Children who are
with externalizing behaviour tend to have low
social thinking. Social thinking or social
cognition refers to how people think about
themselves and others in the social world. The
social cognition construct provides a broad
theoretical perspective that focuses on how people
process information within social contexts. It
includes person perception, causal attributions
concerning self and others, and bringing social
judgments to decision-making, among other
elements. Major difficulties with social cognition
limit their functioning. Deficits in social
cognition lead to behavioural disorders, poor
academic performance, inappropriate interpersonal
relationships, educational and cognitive
impairments, isolation, and psychological issues
[7]. Indian Statistics report revealed that 73% of
conduct disorders have their onset during middle
childhood. Among the Indian studies, Deivasigamani
has reported the prevalence of CD to be 11.13%.
Considering the significance of this problem and
the severe consequences it has in a growing
child’s life we need to understand the different
ways to resolve and prevent a child from growing
into a psychopath.
Studies show that
the development of social cognition has a high
variability concerning individual characteristics
of the child, parent interaction, culture, and
environmental factors [8, 9]. Children with
externalizing disorders lack social cognition
[8,9]. Therefore, it is important to identify
individual and family characteristics that
differentiate children who show persistent
problems from those who follow typical or
changeable pathways.
The present study
aims to explore the attributes of impairment in
social cognition among children with disruptive
behaviours and understand the gaps and lacunae in
areas of existing literature related to this
topic, this scoping review was thought to be
necessary.
Materials and Methods
In order to
understand the current status of research being
conducted in this area, a scoping review of
articles on social cognition among disruptive
children was done. This was done by systematically
using the keywords such as social cognition AND
disruptive children OR anger children OR
externalizing disorders OR conduct disorder and
the timeline selected was from 2008 to 2023.
Articles published on PubMed and Google Scholar
were included in the study. The PRISMA was
followed for reporting the systematic review
(Figure 1).
Inclusion
and Exclusion criteria: Original
articles published in indexed journals were
included. The systematic review, meta-analysis,
general discussions based on secondary data and
qualitative studies were excluded. Studies in
English were considered. Parent responses about
their children were included but teachers’
responses were excluded. Studies on adolescent age
groups, orphans, and shelter/foster care homes
were excluded. Most studies in this field have
been conducted with pubescent youths and adults,
which is why it seems important to study
pre-pubescent populations in order to explore the
developmental origins of aggression. Studies on
children with comorbid conditions like
intellectual disability, genetic anomalies, autism
spectrum disordered, neurological problems,
depression and anxiety were not considered.
Intervention studies were excluded.
Data
collection and Analysis
The various research
mentioned in this review were explored using
scoping review guide by Arksey and O’Malley five
stage methodological framework. At the inception
phase, the finalization of the research question
was done based on articles related to social
cognition published in index journals. In the
second step, a comprehensive search strategy was
developed for 2 electronic citation databases,
Pubmed, and Google scholar using the keywords and
Boolean operators to obtain the maximum number of
articles from 2008 and onwards with the assumption
that these databases index the journals of good
quality. In the third step, as per the
eligibility, systematic review, meta-analysis,
general discussions based on secondary data and
qualitative studies were excluded. Further, in
fourth step, characteristics such as year of
publication, objectives, type and setting, sample
size, study participants, and key findings in the
study were reviewed. Finally, in the fifth step,
all articles were summarized, and frequency
analysis was done. This scoping review was
reported according to the Preferred Reporting
Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses
(PRISMA) extension for Scoping Reviews
(PRISMA-ScR) as shown in the Figure 1.
Ethical Clearance
Ethical clearance was taken from the
Institutional Ethics Committee for further
proceedings.
|
Figure
1: Preferred Reporting Items for
Systematic reviews and Meta Analysis
(PRISMA) for scoping reviews flow diagram
of the search and study selection process
|
Table 1: Results of the Review
|
Study
|
Sample size & Characteristics
|
Tools used
|
Study variable
|
Key Outcomes
|
Role of traits
|
Kolko & Pardini (2010) [10]
|
n=177 children
Age 6 -11years
|
Antisocial Processes Screening Device,
Global Assessment of Functioning, CBCL
|
Role of callous unemotional trait in
children with conduct issues
|
Callous unemotional trait is related to
the empathy deficit and therefore seen in
children with conduct issues
|
Ezpeleta, Granero , de la Osa &
Domènech . (2015) [11]
|
n =622 children
Age 3 -5 years
|
Inventory of Callous-Unemotional Traits,
Children’s Aggression Scale, Social
Communication Disorders Checklist, CBCL
|
Role of callous unemotional trait and ODD
|
High callous unemotional trait was
associated with high anger, externalizing
symptoms of ODD
|
Zumbach, Rademacher & Koglin (2021)
[12]
|
n =371 children, Mean Age=4.7years
|
Inventory of Callous-
Unemotional-Traits
|
Association between Callous unemotional
traits and aggressive behavior
|
Callous unemotional traits is associated
with aggressive behaviors in children
|
Parenting
|
Ziv & Arbel (2020) [13]
|
n=301children
(Mean Age = 5.72 years)
and their mothers
|
-Parenting Styles and Dimensions
Questionnaire,
-Child–parent relationship scale
|
Parenting styles and mother’s attachment
with child in social information
processing of the child
|
Hostile mother results in hostile child.
(Authoritarian parenting style )
|
Chen, Zhou, Eisenberg, Valiente, &
Wang (2011) [14]
|
n=425children Age 7.7-11.6 years &
their parents
|
Self-
Expressiveness in the Family
Questionnaire, Parenting Styles and
Dimensions scale
|
Role of parent expression & parenting
on child’s externalizing behavior
|
Parental negative dominant expressivity
positively predicted Chinese children's
externalizing problems
|
Guajardo, Snyder, & Petersen
(2009)[15]
|
n = 47 children Age 3-5years & their
parents
|
The Parenting Scale (Arnold et al., 1993)
CBCL (Achenbach, 1991, Parent Stress
Index
|
Parenting and social cognition
|
Ineffective parenting lead to misbehavior
and inadequate emotional understanding in
children
|
Bornstein , Putnick, & Suwalsky
(2018)[16]
|
n=317
children below 10 years, & their
mothers
|
Self-Perceptions of the Parental Role,
Parent Attributions Questionnaire,
Preschool Behavior Questionnaire
|
Parental cognition and child’s cognition
|
Better supportive parenting resulted in
low externalizing behavior in children
|
Stoltz, Londen , Dekovic, Prinzie,
Castro, Lochman (2013)[17]
|
n =206 children
Mean Age =10.5 years and their parents
|
Parent version of the Teacher Rating of
Aggression
Alabama Parenting Questionnaire Parenting
Stress Index
|
Parenting and social cognition on
children’s aggressive behavior
|
Negative parenting was related to less
positive self-perception, and resulted in
aggressive tendencies
|
Attachment
|
Licata, Kristen, & Sodian (2016)[18]
|
n =56 children
Age 4years
|
The EAS, Children’s Behavior
Questionnaire
|
Maternal Mind mindedness and child’s
social cognition
|
Emotional availability leads to proper
development of social cognition and
emotional understanding
|
Ziv & Arbel (2020)[13]
|
n=301
152 girls, 149 boys, Mean Age =5.72 years
|
Parenting Styles and Dimensions
Questionnaire ,
Social Information Processing
Questionnaire
|
Role of parenting and mother- child
interaction
|
Maladaptive mother- child relations
results in incompetent social cognition
and social maladjustments in children
|
Bizzi, Ensink, Borelli, Mora, &
Cavanna (2019)[19]
|
n = 131 children, Mean Age =11.2years
|
Child attachment interview, child
reflective functioning scale
CBCL
|
Attachment and mentalizing in children
with disruptive behavior disorders
|
Insecure, disorganized attachment leads
to disruptive behaviour disorder among
children
|
Molina & Musich (2016) [20]
|
n =102 children
Age 7 -13 years
|
Children’s Perception of Parent–Child
Relation Scale, SNAP
|
Child’s perception of parental attachment
and ADHD
|
Controlling and high autonomy in parents
resulted in externalizing symptoms in
children
|
Gender
|
Schultz, Ambike, Logie, Bohner &
Stapleton (2010)[21]
|
n=125
early childhood
|
Schultz Test of Emotion Processing -
Preliminary Version (STEP-P).
|
Role of gender in emotional processing of
a provoking situation
|
Girls were more socially competent than
boys
|
Guajardo, Snyder, & Petersen
(2009)[15]
|
n = 47 children Age 3-5years & their
parents
|
The Parenting Scale (Arnold et al., 1993)
CBCL (Achenbach, 1991, Parent Stress
Index
|
Parenting and social cognition among boys
and girls
|
In boys, there is an inverse relationship
between externalizing behaviour and theory
of mind understanding
|
Salimi, Karimi-Shahanjarini,
Rezapur-Shahkolai, Hamzeh, Roshanaei &
Babamiri(2019) [22]
|
n=900, mean age 11years
|
Researcher developed social cognition and
aggression questionnaire
|
Predicting variable for anger among
elementary schools children
|
Boys tend to have more physical
aggression than girls
|
Executive functions
|
Miranda, Berenguer, Rosello, Baixauli ,
Colomer (2017)[23]
|
n =126
Age 7-11year
|
A Developmental Neuropsychological
Assessment Battery, Theory of Mind
Inventory,
|
analyze social cognition deficits of
children with ADHD
|
Children with ADHD, compared to normal,
showed impairments on all the social
cognition
|
Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive
Function
|
O’Toole , Monks , Tsermentseli (2017)[24]
|
106 children Age 46- and 80-months
|
CGT, BPVS,TOL,
|
relations of EF and ToM to aggression,
|
cool and hot EF and ToM jointly predicted
physical, but not relational,
aggression. In particular, poor cool
inhibition
|
Al-Yagon, Forte, Avrahami
(2020) [25]
|
100, Mean Age 11.4years,
|
Parenting Dimensions Inventory CBCL
|
Role of executive functions on ADHD
|
High deficit in executive functions in
children with ADHD
|
Environmental factors
|
Guajardo, Snyder, & Suwalsky
(2009)[15]
|
n = 47 children Age 3-5years
|
The Parenting Scale (Arnold et al., 1993)
CBCL (Achenbach, 1991, Parent Stress
Index
|
Parenting, parenting stress and social
cognition among children
|
Parenting stress predicts the social
cognition among children. Also results in
externalizing problems in children
|
Hughes, , Aldercotte & Foley
(2017)[26]
|
n =116 children
Age 6 years
|
Alabama Parenting Questionnaire Peer
Relationships Questionnaire e Family
Affluence Scale
|
Maternal Mind-Mindedness and risk for
Disruptive Behavior in Pre-Adolescents in
family adversities
|
Maternal Mind-Mindedness Provides a
Buffer for Pre-Adolescents at Risk for
Disruptive Behavior in case of family
adversities
|
Price, Chiapa, & Walsh (2013)[27]
|
n =177 children, Mean Age =5.43 years
|
CBCL, Post Visit Inventory
|
Social & home environment in
predicting externalizing behaviour
|
Family environment with physical abused
children resulted in externalizing
behavior
|
Renouf , Brendgen , Séguin , Vitaro,
Dionne , Boivin (2010)[28]
|
n=574 children
Age 5years
|
Peer victimization and reactive
aggression
|
High peer victimization resulted in high
reactive aggression
|
|
Silver, Measelle, Armstrong, & Essex
(2010)[29]
|
n= 241 children
Mean Age=4,5 years
|
Preschool Behavior
Questionnaire
Block Child-rearing Practices Report
Child Adaptive Behavior Inventory
|
Teacher and peer victimization
|
Children who were victims of teacher and
peer victimization developed with
externalizing problems
|
Veiga, Neto, & Rieffe (2016)[30]
|
N=78 children Age 55months-77months
|
Free play - connections with emotional
and social functioning
|
Free play and children’s theory of mind
are negatively related to externalizing
behaviors.
|
|
Results
This review was done
on the articles published from 2008 onwards till
2023 (15 years period). The reason for defining
this timeline was to understand how much was done
so far in this area. The information extracted
from the articles included the author, year of
study, sample size, the age range of the sample,
tools used in the study, variables considered in
the study and key findings. Data extraction was
done by the authors independently, and
discrepancies were sorted out through mutual
discussion and consensus. A detailed list of all
the studies included in the present study is given
in Table 1. Studies in English were considered. A
systematic review, meta-analysis and qualitative
studies were excluded. Parent responses about
their children were included but teacher’s
responses were excluded. Studies on adolescent age
groups, orphans, and shelter/foster care homes
were excluded. Studies on children with comorbid
conditions like intellectual disability, genetic
anomalies, autism spectrum disordered,
neurological problems, depression and anxiety were
not considered. Intervention studies were
excluded.
A total of 19360
articles were found on the PubMed database and
18100 articles were found on Google Scholar
relevant for this scoping review. Both databases
were compared, and 423 articles were selected
after removing the duplicates. A total of 423
articles were screened and 391 were removed from
the selection criteria based on eligibility. Out
of 32 full-text articles reviewed, 11 were removed
because of other comorbid conditions present in
children. The present study included 21 articles
for review.
Discussion
Studies which
analyzed children who were brought up by parents,
belonging to early childhood were considered for
the present review. The study considered
elementary and preadolescent age as the theory of
mind or social thinking develops between the age
of 3 to 4 [11].
The results of the
present review are discussed in six categories
based on causal factors or attributes that are
responsible for children with disruptive
behaviours. These are the role of traits,
parenting, attachment, the role of gender,
neuropsychological factors (executive functions)
and environmental factors. In the studies included
in the review, equal participation percentages of
males and females were observed.
Role of Traits
The genes contribute
to a child’s behaviour. The role of a child’s
genetic composition or traits, play a significant
role in contributing to a child’s disruptive
behavior. In the present review, three studies
have been included which emphasize the role of
genetics and trait, leading to the child’s poor
social regulation and externalizing behaviours.
Callous Unemotional (CU) traits characterize a
particular interpersonal and affective style
distinguished by a lack of empathy, remorse, lack
of guilt and constricted feeling. The three
reviewed studies [10, 11, 12] found that children
higher on CU appear to be at increased risk of
experiencing disruptions in parent‐child
attachment relations. Higher CU trait was
associated with high anger, withdrawal and
externalizing symptoms. The presence of CU traits
will alter the appropriate development of the
child which affects his social cues and increases
likelihood of disruptive behavior.
Parenting
Parenting styles
contribute to the impairment in social cognition
and the development of disruptive behaviour among
children. Parenting is expressed in cognitions and
practices followed by a child. Parents’ cognitions
– their parenting knowledge, satisfaction, and
attributions influence the growing child. The
quality of the child’s early family relationships
is critical to our understanding of why some young
children with challenging temperaments show
persistent disruptive behaviour whereas others do
not. Toddlers and preschoolers who manifest high
levels of early disruptive behaviour have been
found to experience lower levels of warm, and
responsive parenting than others [9]. Studies have
revealed that authoritarian parenting styles
contribute to a child’s incompetent response. In
the present review, two studies have found that
authoritarian parenting styles contribute to a
child’s externalizing behaviour. One of these
studies was to understand the role of culture in
understanding parenting style and behavioural
problems among children. The study concluded that
the authoritarian parenting style contributed to a
child’s behaviour problems both in Western and
Asian cultures [13]. Negative parenting or
negative parental emotion toward the children is
another contributory factor to a child’s
externalizing problems. In the present review,
three studies have emphasized that parental
negative dominant expressivity positively
predicted children's externalizing problems.
Inconsistent or harsh discipline leads to a child
developing externalizing or aggressive behaviour.
Across countries, child inhibitory control and
maternal hostility made significant independent
contributions to early externalizing problems, and
leading to disruptive behaviours in young children
[14, 31]
Attachment
Parent–child
attachment plays a significant role in a child who
grows with adequate self-regulation and proper
social cognition. The disruptive pattern in the
parent-child relation results in a child having
disruptive behaviours[18]. In the present review,
three studies have highlighted the role of secure
attachments with the mother as very important in a
growing child. One study revealed that high
parental control and extreme autonomy predicted
externalizing symptoms in children with ADHD [20].
The results showed that low maternal attachment
style contributed to the psychopathology of
children. Mothers who tend to attribute more
hostile intents to unknown others also tend to
view their relationships with their children as
more conflictual than mothers who did not have the
same tendency. In turn, their children are less
competent in social skills. Maternal
mind-mindedness was considered an important
component which brings the emotional connection
between child and mother, which affects the social
cognition of the child. School-aged children with
disruptive behaviours have higher rates of
insecure and disorganized attachment with their
mothers (8,15,18,19)
Gender
Boys’ theory of mind
performance related positively to the severity of
parental discipline, while girls’ performance
related positively to general parental warmth. In
the present review, [15,21] studies highlighted
that girls use their understanding of the mind to
foster relationships with an emphasis on emotional
support, empathy, and cooperation, while boys use
their knowledge in less prosocial ways, leading to
reasons for severely aggressive reactions. In
boys, there is an inverse relationship between
externalizing behaviour and theory of mind [22].
One of the studies revealed that boys enter into
more physical aggression and girls enter into
relational aggression [21]
Executive
functioning
Differences in
executive functions like -attention and inhibitory
control develop rapidly during the toddler and
preschool years and are thought to underlie the
establishment of children’s behavioural
adjustment. The role of the brain and its function
also contribute to a child’s social information
processing or social cognition. In the present
review, three studies have emphasized the role of
executive functions in children with disruptive
behaviours. The involvement of inhibitory control
in different executive function components (e.g.,
affect regulation and motivation) affects a wide
range of behaviours linked to social information
processing. The executive functioning deficit has
been observed in children with externalizing
problems [23,24,25]. One study done specifically
on ADHD children revealed that children with
externalizing problems have impairment in all
social cognition measures- in areas of inhibition,
shifting, emotional control and behavioural
regulation [25].
Environmental
factors
The environmental
factors are mainly referred to any other factors
which caused impairment in social cognition.
Children who are exposed to maltreatment at young
ages are more likely to have impaired
socio-emotional skills and empathy. In the review,
two studies have found that a high level of
adversity in the family leads to poor social
cognition and further contributed to aggressive
behaviours among children [15, 27]. Relative to
non-maltreated children, the family environments
of physically abused children were characterized
by higher levels of negative social interactions.
Also, in comparison to non-maltreated children,
the home environments of children who experienced
neglect were characterized as less organized and
clean [27]. Two studies revealed that high peer
victimization resulted in highly reactive
aggressive behaviour among children [28, 29]. The
absence of free play is another causal factor that
contributes to impaired social cognition. Lack of
free play usually results in disruptive
tendencies, among children. One study has
highlighted the role of free play in building an
adequate level of social understanding among
children [30].
The review has given
a composite view of the attributes or causal
factors which contribute to the impairment of
social cognition among disruptive children. Though
the scarcity in the number of studies limits the
generalizability of the review results, it serves
to provide a reliable overview of the subject with
the existing evidence. When one plans for a
treatment or intervention for children with
disruptive behaviours, the role of social
cognition and its enhancement should be considered
as an inevitable area of work for improvement as
it is one of the major deficits in these children.
Considering the increasing prevalence of anger
among children, it is essential to examine the
different factors that influence a child’s
disruptive behaviour. To our knowledge, this is
the first attempt at reviewing the multicomponent
which are responsible for causing disruptive
behaviour among children.
The 21 reviewed
studies included approximately 5693 children below
12 years. Methodological quality varied, but every
study had its limitations. Each study has looked
into one particular factor which is responsible
for poor social cognition among children with
disruptive or externalizing behaviours. However,
we should understand that not every factor is
responsive to cause impairment in social cognition
among children with disruptive behaviour, but one
should consider these entire components while
treating a child with disruptive behaviours.
Conclusion
In conclusion, the
aim of the present review was to synthesize
up-to-date evidence about understanding the
multicomponent responsible for causing aggression
among children as it is getting alarmingly
prevalent. Understanding the social cognitive
theory as the framework for the prediction of
aggression behaviours in children and adolescents
can be used in designing and implementing
educational interventions in the management of
aggression in children and adolescents. Earlier
specific social adjustment difficulties are
detected in younger children, there is a scope for
planning effective treatment or intervention to
cure this at the early stage of life. Accordingly,
we can plan early intervention programs aiming to
reduce children’s problem behaviour and increase
their prosocial behaviours as early as possible.
Our findings could make an important contribution
to such programs because this review identifies
different paths by which early maladjusted
behaviours develop in a child. In particular, the
findings that social maladjustments are associated
with biased and incompetent social cognition
should be highlighted in that respect.
Limitations
of the study
The main limitation
of the present review is the lack of an adequate
number of studies available in the published
literature on the subject. We might have missed
articles indexed on other databases.
Future
research, implications and recommendations
Future research must
focus on considering an intervention which looks
into the different factors which are responsible
for anger in children, by using social cognition
and parent-mediated intervention. Knowledge about
risk factors for understanding externalizing
behaviour in children displaying symptoms of
behavioural disorders can result in more adequate
preventive indicated interventions. The findings
from this study indicate that externalizing
behaviour problems apparent in early elementary
school are associated with experiences occurring
prior to and concurrent with the transition into
elementary school. An important direction for
future research will be to determine which all
contextual factors contribute to the onset of
externalizing problems and which factors
contribute to their maintenance. This type of
research will have important implications for the
prevention and treatment of externalizing
behaviour problems. Aggression in children and
adolescents can be prevented and reduced through
designing and implementing educational
interventions based on these factors.
Conflict of Interest:
None declared
References
- Moretti MM, Holland R, McKay S. Self-other
representations and relational and overt
agression in adolescent girls and boys. Behav.
Sci. Law 2001;19:109–126.
- Venta A, Hatkevich C, Mellick W, Vanwoerden S,
Sharp C. Social cognition mediates the relation
between attachment schemas and posttraumatic
stress disorder. Psychol. Trauma Theory Res.
Pract. Policy 2017;9: 88–95.
- Erskine HE, Norman RE, Ferrari AJ, Chan GCK,
Copeland WE, Whiteford HA et al. Long-Term
Outcomes of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity
Disorder and Conduct Disorder: A Systematic
Review and Meta-Analysis. Journal of the
American Academy of Child and Adolescent
Psychiatry 2016;55(10): 841-850.
- American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic
and statistical manual of mental disorders
(DSM-IV). American Psychiatric Pub. 1994
- Speranzini N, Goodarzi Z, Casselman L,
Pringsheim T. Barriers and Facilitators
Associated with the Management of Aggressive and
Disruptive Behaviour in Children: A Qualitative
Study with Pediatricians. J Can Acad Child
Adolesc Psychiatry. 2020;29(3):177-187
- Abrahamse ME, Junger M, van Wouwe, MAMM, Boer
F, Lindauer RJL. Treating Child Disruptive
Behavior in High-Risk Families: A Comparative
Effectiveness Trial from a Community-Based
Implementation. J Child Fam Stud
2016;25:1605–1622.
- O’Connor E, Rodriguez E, Cappella E, Morris J,
McClowry S. Child Disruptive Behaviour and
parenting efficacy: A comparison of the effects
of two models of Insights. J Community 2012;40(5):
555–572.
- Olson, SL, Choe DE, Sameroff AJ. Trajectories
of child externalizing problems between ages 3
and 10 years: Contributions of children's early
effortful control, theory of mind, and parenting
experiences. Dev Psychopathol 2017;29(4):1333-1351
- Benarous X, Guile JM, Consoli A, Cohen D. A
systematic review of the evidence for impaired
cognitive theory of mind in maltreated children.
Frontiers in Psychiatry 2015;6:108.119
- Kolko DJ, Pardini DA. ODD dimensions, ADHD,
and callous– unemotional traits as predictors of
treatment response in children with disruptive
behavior disorders. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology 2010;119(4):713-25.
- Ezpeleta L, Granero R, de la Osa N, Domenech
JM (2015) Clinical Characteristics of Preschool
Children with Oppositional Defiant Disorder and
Callous-Unemotional Traits. PLoS ONE
2015;10(9): e0139346.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0139346.
- Zumbach J, Rademacher A, Koglin U.
Conceptualizing callous‑unemotional traits in
preschoolers: Associations with social‑emotional
competencies and aggressive behavior. Child
Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health
2021;15:24-34
- Ziv Y, Arbel R. Association between the
Mother's Social Cognition and the Child's Social
Functioning in Kindergarten: The Mediating Role
of the Child's Social Cognition. International
Journal of Environmental research and Public
Health 2020;17(1). Available at https://www.mdpi.com/1660-4601/17/1/358
- Chen SH, Zhou Q, Eisenberg N, Valiente C, Wang
Y. Parental expressivity and parenting styles in
Chinese families: Prospective and unique
relations to children's psychological
adjustment. Parent Sci Pract 2011;11(4):288-
307
- Guajardo NR, Snyder G Petersen R.
Relationships among parenting practices,
parental stress, child behaviour, and children's
social-cognitive development. Infant and
Child Development 2009;18(1):37–60.
- Bornstein MH, Putnick DL, Suwalsky
JTD. Parenting cognitions→ parenting practices→
child adjustment?: The standard model. Dev
Psychopathol2018;30(2): 399–416.
- Stoltz, S. Londen M, Dekovic M, Prinzie
P, Castro B, Lochman JE. Simultaneously Testing
Parenting and Social Cognitions in Children
At-Risk for Aggressive Behavior Problems: Sex
Differences and Ethnic Similarities. Journal
of Child and Family Studies
2013;22(7):922-931.
- Licata M, Kristen S, Sodian B. Mother–child
interaction as a cradle of theory of mind: the
role of maternal emotional availability. Social
Development 2016;25 (1): 139-156
- Bizzi F, Ensink K, Borelli JL, Mora SC,
Cavanna D. Attachment and reflective functioning
in children with somatic symptom disorders and
disruptive behavior disorders. Eur Child
Adolesc Psychiatry 2019;28(5): 705-717.
- Molina MF, Musich FM. Perception of parenting
style by children with ADHD and its relation
with inattention, hyperactivity/impulsivity and
externalizing symptoms. Journal of Child and
Family Studies. 2016;25(5):1656–1671.
- Schultz D, Ambike A, Logie SK, Bohner KE,
Stapleton LM et al. Assessment of social
information processing in early childhood:
Development and initial validation of the
Schultz test of emotion processing—Preliminary
version. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2010;38(5):
601-13.
- Salimi N, Karimi-Shahanjarini A,
Rezapur-Shahkolai F, Hamzeh B, Roshanaei G,
Babamiri M. Aggression and its predictors among
elementary students. Journal of Injury and
Violence Research. 2019;11(2):159–170.
- Miranda A, Berenguer C, Rosello B, Baixauli I,
Colomer C. Social Cognition in Children with
High-Functioning Autism Spectrum Disorder and
Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder.
Associations with Executive Functions. Front.
Psychol 2017; 8:1035.
- O’Toole SE, Monks CP, Tsermentseli S.
Executive function and theory of mind as
predictors of aggressive and prosocial behavior
and peer acceptance in early childhood. Social
Development. 2017;26(4):907–920.
- Al-Yagon M, Forte D, Avrahami L. Executive
functions and attachment relationships in
children with ADHD: Links to
externalizing/internalizing problems, social
skills, and negative mood regulation. J
Atten Disord 2020, 24(13):1876-1890.
- Hughes C, Aldercotte A, Foley S. Maternal
mind-mindedness provides a buffer for
pre-adolescents at risk for disruptive behavior.
J Abnorm Child Psychol 2017;45 (2):
225-235.
- Price JM, Chiapa A, Walsh NE. Predictors of
externalizing behavior problems in early
elementary-aged children: the role of family and
home environments. The Journal of Genetic
Psychology 2013;174(4):464–471.
- Renouf A, Brendgen M, Séguin JR, Vitaro F,
Dionne G, Boivin M et al. Interactive links
between theory of mind, peer victimization, and
reactive and proactive aggression. J Abnorm
Child Psychol 2010;38:1109-1123
- Silver RB, Measelle JR, Armstrong JM, Essex
MJ. The impact of parents, child care providers,
teachers, and peers on early externalizing
trajectories. Journal of School Psychology
2010;48(6): 555–583.
- Veiga G, Neto C, Rieffe C. Preschoolers’ free
play - connections with emotional and social
functioning. International Journal of
Emotional Education 2016;8(1): 48-62.
- Sen A, Mukherjee T. A study of personality
dimensions, perception of parents and parenting
style in conduct disorder children. Indian
Journal of Clinical Psychology 2012;39
(1): 34-41.
|